"The role of executive functions in emotion regulation" тема диссертации и автореферата по ВАК РФ 19.00.01, кандидат наук Мохаммед Абдул-Рахеем

  • Мохаммед Абдул-Рахеем
  • кандидат науккандидат наук
  • 2022, ФГАОУ ВО «Национальный исследовательский университет «Высшая школа экономики»
  • Специальность ВАК РФ19.00.01
  • Количество страниц 132
Мохаммед Абдул-Рахеем. "The role of executive functions in emotion regulation": дис. кандидат наук: 19.00.01 - Общая психология, психология личности, история психологии. ФГАОУ ВО «Национальный исследовательский университет «Высшая школа экономики». 2022. 132 с.

Оглавление диссертации кандидат наук Мохаммед Абдул-Рахеем

Table of Contents

General Introduction

Chapter 1: Executive functions as a possible cognitive basis of emotion regulation (Literature Review)

1.1 The concept of emotion regulation

1.1.2 Focus on cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression

1.1.3 Assessment of emotion regulation strategies: self-reports versus laboratory studies

1.2 The concept of executive functions

1.2.1 Updating

1.2.2 Shifting

1.2.3 Inhibition

1.2.4 Non-affective and affective executive functions

1.3 The relationship between executive functions and emotion regulation

1.3.1 Updating and emotion regulation

1.3.2 Shifting and emotion regulation

1.3.3 Inhibition and emotion regulation

1.3.4 Summary of the studies of the relationship between executive functions and emotion regulation

1.4 The present research

Chapter 2: A study of non-affective and affective executive functions

2.1 Rationale and hypothesis

2.2 Method

2.2.1 Participants

3

2.2.2 Stimuli

2.2.3 Task design

2.2.4 Procedure

2.2.5 Data preparation

2.3 Results

2.4 Discussion

Chapter 3: An examination of the effectiveness of emotion regulation strategies: self-reports versus laboratory studies

3.1 Rationale and hypotheses

3.2 Method

3.2.1 Participants

3.2.2 Materials and study design

3.2.3 Data collection and reduction

3.2.4 Data analyses

3.3 Results

3.4 Discussion

Chapter 4: An examination of the role of executive functions in emotion regulation strategies

4.1 Rationale

4.2 Study

4.2.1 Method

4.2.2 Results

4.2.3 Discussion

4.3 Study

4.3.1 Method

4.3.3 Results

4.3.4 Discussion

4.4 General Discussion

Summary and Conclusions

References

List of figures

List of tables

Appendices

Appendix A. ERQ questionnaire

Abbreviations

Acknowledgments

Рекомендованный список диссертаций по специальности «Общая психология, психология личности, история психологии», 19.00.01 шифр ВАК

Введение диссертации (часть автореферата) на тему «"The role of executive functions in emotion regulation"»

General Introduction

Research problem

Emotion regulation (also referred to as emotion self-regulation) encompasses the processes and strategies that are involved in the modulation of experience and expressions of emotions (Gross, 1998, 2015; McRae & Gross, 2020). The regulation of an emotion could occur at any stage of the emotion trajectory. This could be in the form of down-regulation (aimed at reducing the intensity of emotional experience) or upregulation (aimed at increasing the intensity of emotional experience). Different aspects of emotion could be subjected to some form of regulation such as subjective experience, valence of the emotion, emotional expressions, and physiological responses related to emotions (Gross, 1998).

The ability to regulate emotions cannot be underestimated. This topic has become important considering its role in psychosocial wellbeing. It is well documented that a higher ability to regulate one's emotions is associated with important positive health outcomes such as better mental health and psychosocial wellbeing (Gross, 2014; Tamir, 2016; Verzeletti et al., 2016). Conversely, outcomes of poor emotion regulation are associated with depressive symptoms (Joormann & Stanton, 2016; Visted et al., 2018), poor psychosocial wellbeing (Nasso et al., 2019), and poor mental health (Aldao et al., 2010; Mennin & Farach, 2007).

Although many strategies of emotion regulation are described in the literature, not all are considered adaptive. For instance, rumination (Aker et al., 2014; Besharat et al., 2013; Zawadzki, 2015), worry (Knepp et al., 2015; Neudert et al., 2017), and excessive use of suppression (Appleton et al., 2013; Cai et al., 2019; Nittel et al., 2018) have been considered as maladaptive emotion regulation strategies. Extreme forms of maladaptive strategies, which have both short-term and long-term impacts on physical health, include excessive use of alcohol and drugs (Dvorak et al., 2014; Petit et al., 2015).

The most influential model of emotion regulation is the process model by Gross (1998, 2015). This model classifies emotion regulation strategies based on the time-course of

emotion generation. The notable distinction is the antecedent-focused strategies (e.g., cognitive reappraisal) and response-focused strategies (e.g., expressive suppression).

The implementation of emotion regulation strategies is considered to have cognitive roots (Schmeichel & Tang, 2015; Suri et al., 2013). It has further been posited that higher cognitive abilities could subserve successful application of emotion regulation strategies (Tull & Aldao, 2015). Hence, cognitive processes which include executive functions are thought to be at the heart of emotion regulation. As a result, more efficient executive functions are expected to result in better emotion regulation outcomes. This argument, however, has received little empirical support so far.

Executive functions (also referred to as cognitive control or executive control) are broadly defined as higher-order cognitive processes that modulate the functioning of the processes at low-level cognition (Friedman & Miyake, 2017; Miyake et al., 2000). Executive functions subserve the control and regulation of thought and action repertoire (Friedman et al., 2006). The prefrontal cortex, caudate nucleus, and subthalamic nucleus (brain regions), which are associated with executive processes, also play a mediating role in inhibitory control (Alvarez & Emory, 2006; Diamond, 2013). Although several models have been developed to explain executive functions, the most acclaimed among them is the unity-diversity framework (Miyake et al., 2000), which posits three types of executive functions: updating, shifting, and inhibition.

Several studies investigated the relationship between executive functions and cognitive abilities such as planning (Miyake et al., 2000), maintenance of goals (Kane & Engle, 2003), and dual tasking (D'esposito et al., 1995). Individual differences in executive functions in a healthy population and their role in psychopathology have received considerable attention. Other factors that influence executive functions have been investigated, too. These include the influence of mood on executive functions. For instance, an intense negative emotion undermines inhibitory control, while a milder negative emotion improves inhibitory control (Pessoa et al., 2012). While negative mood is argued to impair

7

working memory updating (Curci et al., 2013), positive mood facilitates the ability to switch from one task to another, although accompanied by an increased distraction (Dreisbach & Goschke, 2004).

There is paucity of research on the relationship between executive functioning and emotion regulation. Specific ways in which executive functions relate to emotion regulation strategies are poorly understood. This can be attributed to different and not directly comparable operationalizations of executive functions as well as of emotion regulation. In most of the studies, a higher level of updating is positively associated with cognitive reappraisal strategy of emotion regulation but not with expressive suppression (Pe et al., 2013a, 2013b, 2015; Schmeichel et al., 2008). Positive (Liang et al., 2017), negative (McRae et al., 2012), and no association (Malooly et al., 2013; Sperduti et al., 2017) have been reported in the relationship between shifting and emotion regulation strategies. The majority of the studies failed to find a relationship between inhibition and suppression strategy of emotion regulation (Aker et al., 2014; Hendricks & Buchanan, 2016; McRae et al., 2012). One study reported a positive association between inhibition and cognitive reappraisal (Cohen & Mor, 2018). Whereas some studies examined executive functions using affective stimuli (e.g., Pe et al., 2015), others did not (e.g., Hendricks & Buchanan, 2016; Xiu et al., 2016). It can be suggested that since emotion regulation is based on affective information, executive functions that deal with emotional content may be more linked to emotion regulation strategies. Emotion regulation has been assessed using different measures and focusing on strategies which also leads to inconsistent findings (e.g., Gyurak et al., 2012; Sperduti et al., 2017).

The main aim of this PhD thesis is to explore the relationship between executive functions and emotion regulation strategies. This includes the development of the non-affective and affective tasks measuring executive functions. This would enable one to understand the extent to which non-affective and affective contents in executive functions are related to emotion regulation strategies. The thesis further seeks to examine the

effectiveness of two main emotion regulation strategies, cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression, measured via self-report and psychophysiological responses. This would allow one to understand how executive functions contribute to the successful implementation of emotion regulation strategies.

Objectives of the research

1. To analyze the literature on executive functions and emotion regulation.

2. To develop a battery of executive function tasks using affective and non-affective content.

3. To develop experimental procedures for measuring emotion regulation in the laboratory.

4. To compare different measures of the effectiveness of emotion regulation strategies.

5. To examine the relationship between executive functions and emotion regulation.

General hypothesis

Executive functions are the cognitive basis of emotion regulation and therefore predict successful implementation of emotion regulation strategies.

Specific hypotheses

1. There will be a positive association between executive functions processing non-affective and affective content.

2. Reduction of negative emotional state by the emotion regulation strategies would manifest itself in self-report, facial expressions, and autonomic responses.

3. Cognitive reappraisal would result in a larger reduction in negative emotions than in expressive suppression.

4. Updating would be positively associated with cognitive reappraisal.

5. Inhibition would be positively associated with expressive suppression.

6. Shifting ability would be related to more effective use of cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression.

7. Executive functions processing affective content would be more strongly associated with emotion regulation compared to executive functions processing non-affective content.

Research methodology

Overall, 2281 participants (mean age = 20.55, SD = 3.41, female = 61.3%) were recruited for the experiments. They were largely student volunteers who took part in the experiments in exchange for course credits. The participants signed informed consent before taking part in the experiments. All experiments were approved by the Institutional Review Board of the Higher School of Economics.

An experimental method was used for the studies. N-back, letter-number, and Stroop tasks were used to measure updating, shifting, and inhibition respectively. Emotion regulation strategies were assessed using an emotion regulation task accompanied by self-reports, electromyography (EMG) of zygomaticus major and corrugator supercilii, skin conductance response (SCR), heart rate (HR) measurement, and the Gross Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ). These tasks were carried out at the behavioural and electroencephalography (EEG) laboratories of the Centre for Cognition and Decision Making, Higher School of Economics. The experimental stimuli were obtained largely from two sources, the EU-Emotion stimulus set (O'Reilly et al., 2012, 2015) and the International Affective Picture System (IAPS: Lang et al., 1997).

ActiChamp (Brain Products, Germany), a data collecting system, was used to extract and filter the psychophysiological data during the emotion regulation task.

1 This is comprised of 84 in Chapter 2, 64 in Chapter 3, 63 and 81 in Chapter 4. However, the sample in Chapter 3 was part of the sample in Chapter 4, Study 2.

Statistical analysis included, mixed-design analysis of variance (ANOVA), correlational tests, Friedman's analysis, Benjamini and Hochberg's (1995) correction for false discovery rate, Cliff's 5 (Cliff, 1993), and /-tests. The statistical tests were carried out using IBM SPSS 21.0, JASP 0.13.1.0, and STATISTICA 12.

Scientific novelty

The PhD thesis makes a contribution to further scientific knowledge on mechanisms of emotion regulation. Attempts were made in the past to examine the relationship between executive functions and emotion regulation which produced inconsistent findings. The current thesis extends this knowledge by fine-tuning the operationalization of these concepts to understand how executive functions contribute to emotion regulation success. Thus, for the first time, executive functions were measured using tasks containing non-affective and affective content. This led to the development of a new battery of tasks measuring executive functions. Although Pessoa (2009) posited that affective content in tasks does have an impact on executive functioning, this was not supported in the present research. Hence, the present findings support the view that affective content in these tasks does not influence performance in a healthy population (cf. Schweizer et al., 2019).

Emotion regulation strategies were also examined using multiple measures. Self-reports, measures of peripheral psychophysiology, and questionnaires were among the methods used. Since the experience and expression of emotions can be observed via self-report and psychophysiological responses, the emotion regulation strategies were assessed from these outcomes as well. The current findings add to what is already known on the association between executive functions and emotion regulation. The evidence of the positive association between updating and cognitive reappraisal supports the growing body of research that reported similar results. For the first time, the ability to inhibit automatic responses was demonstrated to be related to the frequent use of suppression of emotional expression. The results provided further evidence that emotion regulation strategies differ based on the measure used during the assessment. Within the framework of the process

model of emotion regulation (Gross, 1998), the current findings contribute to the distinction between cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression. Self-reported unpleasant emotion was shown to be reduced in greater extent by cognitive reappraisal than by expressive suppression. These findings could explain the previous inconsistent findings on the relationship between executive functions and emotion regulation.

The theoretical significance of this research is the contribution to the refinement of existing theories such as the dual-competition framework (Pessoa, 2009) and the process model of emotion regulation (Gross, 1998). According to the dual-competition framework (Pessoa, 2009), executive functions are influenced by affective content. However, based on the results of the present research and recent studies (e.g., Schweizer et al., 2019), it is suggested that the influence of affective content in executive functioning is not prevalent in a healthy population but in individuals with affective disorders. According to the process model of emotion regulation (Gross, 1998), cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression differ according to their temporal application and their consequences. This distinction has received empirical support across multiple measures.

The practical implication of the research is a potential application of the present findings in clinical psychology, cognitive psychology, and cognitive neuroscience as well. The present findings would also be important for the content of future textbooks or manuals in these areas. As executive functions and emotion regulation feature in these areas, the present findings could be incorporated into what is already known about these topics and their relationship. As higher inhibitory control leads to the successful implementation of suppression, training on inhibition could help improve suppression in emotion regulation. Clinicians, in their efforts at helping people with emotion dysregulation, could resort to training programs aimed at improving executive functions. Hence, one possible target for improving emotion regulation is executive functions.

Statements for the defense

1. Some executive functions can be considered as the cognitive basis of emotion

regulation. Updating is positively associated with the effectiveness of the cognitive

12

reappraisal strategy of emotion regulation, whereas inhibition is positively associated with the frequency of the use of the expressive suppression strategy of emotion regulation.

2. Cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression are effective in downregulating negative emotion. However, the effectiveness of emotion regulation strategies manifests itself differently depending on the type of measures used for assessment. Differences between cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression are greatest in self-report and heart rate deceleration measures.

3. Executive functions, namely updating, shifting, and inhibition, do not differ with respect to affective and non-affective content. Non-affective and affective measures of executive functions yield similar patterns of associations with measures of emotion regulation strategies.

Approbation and introduction of results

The outcome and content of this research were discussed at various seminars and scientific conferences, as listed below.

Conferences:

1. Society for Affective Science virtual meeting - Gater.Town. April 13-16, 2021. Oral presentation: The role of executive functions in emotion regulation.

2. Society for Affective Science virtual meeting - Gater.Town. April 13-16, 2021. Oral presentation: Emotion regulation strategies depending on empathy: Psychophysiological study.

3. International forum, Cognitive neuroscience. December 11-12, 2020, Yekaterinburg, Russia. Poster presentation: The role of executive functions in emotion regulation.

4. Psy-HSE: Conference of Young Scientists. Contemporary Issues of Psychological Science. October 31-November 2, 2019. Moscow, Russia. Oral presentation: Individual differences in executive functions and emotion regulation.

5. International Society for Research on Emotion. July 10-13, 2019. Amsterdam. Flash talk and poster presentation: Updating of emotional stimuli in working memory: The role of mood and emotion regulation.

6. 5th Conference "Cognitive Science in Moscow: New Research". July 19, 2019. Poster presentation: The role of induced mood and emotion regulation in updating of valenced information.

The content of the dissertation is presented in three published articles. They are listed below:

1. Mohammed, A.-R., Kosonogov, V., & Lyusin, D. (2021). Expressive suppression versus cognitive reappraisal: Effects on self-report and peripheral psychophysiology. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 167, 30-37. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2021.06.007

2. Mohammed, A.-R. & Lyusin D. (2020). The role of affective updating in the cognitive reappraisal strategy of emotion regulation. Psychological Thought, 13(2), 302-321. https://doi.org/10.37708/psyct.v13i2.515

3. Mohammed, A.-R. (2019). A battery of cognitive tasks for the assessment of non-affective and affective executive functioning. The Russian Journal of Cognitive Science, 6(3), 38-49.

Structure of the dissertation

Chapter 1 includes the review of the present literature on executive functions and emotion regulation including their assessments. Chapter 2 describes the development and analysis of the new battery of tasks measuring non-affective and affective executive functions. Chapter 3 presents the assessment of the effectiveness of emotion regulation strategies. Chapter 4 describes the study of the relationship between executive functions and emotion regulation. A summary of the findings and the general discussion including the conclusions were also provided.

Похожие диссертационные работы по специальности «Общая психология, психология личности, история психологии», 19.00.01 шифр ВАК

Заключение диссертации по теме «Общая психология, психология личности, история психологии», Мохаммед Абдул-Рахеем

Summary and Conclusions

The relevant role emotion regulation plays cannot be underestimated. The important contribution of emotion regulation in psychosocial wellbeing and the attainment of social goals is well documented (Gross, 2014; Tamir, 2016; Verzeletti et al., 2016). The ability to successfully engage in any emotion regulation activity is said to be influenced by cognitive factors such as executive functions (Schmeichel & Tang, 2015; Suri et al., 2013). Although several executive functions have been mentioned in the literature, converging research postulates that there are three executive functions that are separate at the component level but unite to form a complex executive functions model (Friedman & Miyake, 2017; Miyake et al., 2000). They are updating, shifting, and inhibition. However, the specific ways in which these executive functions contribute to emotion regulation strategies are poorly understood.

To explore the various ways through which executive functions are related to emotion regulation, executive functions and emotion regulation were examined separately. This involved the theoretical underpinnings of executive functions and the role of affective content in tasks measuring executive functions. Emotion regulation was investigated by highlighting the models and the strategies adopted in the regulation of emotion including their assessment. Next, was the investigation of the specific ways executive functions were related to emotion regulation strategies. Thus, specific executive functions (i.e., updating, shifting, and inhibition) were explored in relation to two emotion regulation strategies (i.e., cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression).

These objectives were achieved by presenting the state of the literature on executive functions and emotion regulation (Chapter 1). Here, the various theoretical models of executive functions and emotion regulation including the tasks often used in their examination were reviewed. The revision of the literature also included empirical studies conducted on executive functions processing non-affective and affective materials, assessment of emotion regulation strategies, and the association between executive functions and emotion regulation strategies. This brought to light the general impression on the state

of research on executive functions and emotion regulation. The next stage involved various empirical studies. Specifically, a new battery of tasks was developed to examine how affective content influences the tasks measuring executive functions (Chapter 2). The assessment of emotion regulation strategies was also explored to understand how the outcome of these strategies are manifested in self-report and peripheral psychophysiological responses (Chapter 3). Having dissected executive functions and emotion regulation separately, this allowed for the examination of the relationship between executive functions and emotion regulation strategies (Chapter 4). Taking all together, each of the four empirical studies aimed to clarify the nature of executive functions and emotion regulation and the specific ways they are related. Specifically, the research questions that were intended to be answered in the various chapters of the thesis were:

1. What are the theoretical underpinnings of executive functions and emotion regulation? What is the current state of the literature on the association between executive functions and emotion regulation? (Chapter 1);

2. Does affective content in the tasks measuring executive functions influence the performances of executive functions? Does it occur across all executive functions or is it peculiar to specific executive functions? (Chapter 2);

3. How effective are cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression strategies? What is the comparative effectiveness of these strategies when assessed via self-report and psychophysiological measures? Specifically, do the emotion regulation strategies show similar outcomes as manifested in self-reports and psychophysiological responses? (Chapter 3);

4. What is the relationship between executive functions and emotion regulation strategies? Will measures of executive functions in affective tasks be more related to emotion regulation strategies; or the affective content in the executive functions task does not matter in this relationship? (Chapter 4).

To have a general insight into the research questions formulated above, the literature reviewed provided more information on the existing studies of executive functions and emotion regulation (Chapter 1), this led to the development of a new battery of tasks measuring executive functions processing non-affective and affective information (Chapter 2). An experiment was also conducted on the effectiveness of emotion regulation strategies using self-report and psychophysiological responses (Chapter 3). This allowed to directly estimate the relationship between executive functions and emotion regulation strategies (Chapter 4).

In the next sections, the main findings of the systematic review of literature coupled with the four empirical studies conducted are summarized. These studies would be described in terms of how they address the research questions and their contribution to the outline of this dissertation. The theoretical and practical implications including the limitations and recommendations for future studies are provided as well.

Main findings and conclusions

In Chapter 1, the analysis of the literature on executive functions processing affective content showed that executive functions are impaired in individuals with affective disorders. A large body of research showed that individuals with varying degrees of affective disorders also report disruptions of affective content in executive functions. Little is known about executive functions processing non-affective and affective materials in a healthy population. The review conducted on the effectiveness of emotion regulation strategies showed discrepancies in the type of measure deployed. While self-report measures consistently showed cognitive reappraisal as the most effective strategy, mixed findings were reported on the use of psychophysiological measures. The use of varied paradigms in the assessment of the effectiveness of emotion regulation strategies could explain the pattern of results obtained. Limited research characterized the relationship between executive functions and emotion regulation. The role of affect was not given the needed attention in the association between executive functions and emotion regulation.

The main findings on executive function processing non-affective and affective content were reported in Chapter 2. No mean differences were observed between the performance related to non-affective and affective versions of updating, shifting, and inhibition tasks. Medium positive correlations between the non-affective and affective versions of the updating and inhibition tasks were also observed. This suggests that both versions capture similar psychological constructs (i.e., executive functions), although they are not identical. The results of the study do not support the dual-competition framework (Pessoa, 2009) which posits that affective content influences executive functions.

In Chapter 3, a comparison of the measures of the emotion regulation strategies was carried out. Specifically, self-reports and psychophysiological measures of cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression were examined. The difference between cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression was observed only in self-report and HR deceleration. In all the measures used to assess emotion regulation strategies, different patterns of the effectiveness of the emotion regulation strategies were observed. This suggests that the use of multiple measures in the assessment of emotion regulation strategies would be more appropriate.

The main findings on the relationship between executive functions and emotion regulation are reported in Chapter 4. The two experiments showed that higher updating of affective information was related to the cognitive reappraisal strategy of emotion regulation. Inhibition was associated with the more frequent use of expressive suppression. Shifting was associated with neither cognitive reappraisal nor expressive suppression. Finally, the affective content of the tasks when measuring executive functions did not influence the relationship between executive functions and emotion regulation strategies.

A novel battery of executive functions tasks with non-affective and affective content was created. Although the individual tasks were not new, there is no existing battery of tasks that measures updating, shifting, and inhibition by incorporating non-affective and affective

content in these tasks. The novel battery contained the n-back, letter-number, and Stroop tasks.

The present thesis provided data on the outcome of emotion regulation strategies measured via self-reports, behavioural, and psychophysiological responses. We were able to observe how a singular emotional reaction is manifested via self-reports, facial expressions (EMG of corrugator supercilii and zygomaticus major), and autonomic responses (SCR and HR). The data also showed that there is greater HR deceleration during the suppression of negative emotions. The assessment of the effectiveness of emotion regulation strategies in previous studies failed to examine the outcome in the three main pathways of emotional expressions (i.e., experiential, behavioural, and physiological reaction).

Unlike previous studies, the present research examined the relationship between executive functions and emotion regulation by using both non-affective and affective tasks measuring executive functions. This made it possible to investigate whether non-affective or affective tasks measuring executive functions are more related to emotion regulation. For the first time, inhibition was found to be positively associated with the frequent use of the expressive suppression strategy of emotion regulation.

The review of the literature and the results of the four empirical studies conducted have important theoretical implications. Although the dual-competition framework (Pessoa, 2009) posited the influence of affective content in tasks performance, the present findings do not support this assertion. Indeed, the absence of differences observed between non-affective and affective tasks measuring executive functions could be more prominent in a healthy population, as reported in a recent study (Schweizer et al., 2019). Thus, affect does not influence executive functions in a healthy population.

The outcome of the study on emotion regulation strategies gives further insight into the process model of emotion regulation (Gross, 1998, 2015). The effectiveness of cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression in reducing negative emotions differed depending on the type of measurement used. Since cognitive reappraisal is applied at the early stage of

95

emotion generation (i.e., during the interpretation stage), the representation of the affective stimulus is at the conscious stage hence cognitive reappraisal should be more effective than suppression. This was confirmed using the self-report. As a result, cognitive reappraisal is more effective in changing subjective experience because it is less cognitively costly. The pattern of results obtained requires that the investigation of the effectiveness of emotion regulation strategies should deploy multiple measures (i.e., subjective experience, facial expressions, and psychophysiological response).

In this research, some theoretical ideas were proposed regarding the specific ways executive functions should relate to emotion regulation strategies. To successfully reappraise an emotional stimulus would require an efficient updating ability, as updating would involve monitoring and manipulation of the emotional stimulus in working memory. This assumption has received partial support from the findings of the present research.

Shifting, which includes the back-and-forth attentional movement between or among multiple tasks and the internal control mechanism that facilitates the prevention of previous task rules from interfering in the subsequent task rules should be relevant for both cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression. While the back-and-forth attentional movement in shifting enables one to engage in several tasks at a time, this would be beneficial to cognitive reappraisal due to its requirement to engage in alternative interpretation of an emotional stimulus. Additionally, the internal control mechanism that helps to overcome interference from previous task rules would facilitate the ability to engage in expressive suppression. However, the results of the present study did not support the association between shifting and the two emotion regulation strategies.

As expressive suppression involves inhibiting an ongoing emotional behaviour, this should be subserved by inhibition. This idea has been supported by the present findings on the relationship between inhibition and expressive suppression. Finally, it was postulated that executive functions processing affective content would be more associated with emotion regulation strategies. This is premised on the notion that as emotion regulation deals with

affective information, affective executive functions should predict emotion regulation success. This idea was however not supported by the results of the present research.

This research has far-reaching implications for areas such as cognitive psychology, clinical psychology, and cognitive neuroscience. The textbooks of these subjects could be revised with the new findings obtained in this research. The present findings on emotion regulation show that the manifestation of emotion experience occurs in three main outlets (i.e., self-reports, facial expressions, and physiological responses). To examine the effectiveness of emotion regulation strategies, it is important to consider multiple measures. This is important for clinicians as they engage their clients in emotion regulation training. For instance, the deployment of HR sensors in addition to the self-report measures during emotion regulation training would go a long way to provide evidence regarding the changes in an emotional state.

The ability to regulate one's emotion is important for the attainment of subjective wellbeing and social goals. In order to be effective at emotion regulation, efforts are made to educate or train people on how to regulate their emotions. The outcome of this research points to the facilitatory role of executive functions in the implementation of emotion regulation strategies. This means that training manuals that aim to improve emotion regulation should pay an attention to executive functions. Based on the results of this research, it is expected that the improvement of executive functions should play an enabling role in emotion regulation abilities.

Limitations and recommendations for further studies

Even though the thesis has made contributions to the understanding of the relationship between executive functions and emotion regulation, the study has some limitations. The sample consisted largely of undergraduate students and younger adults. Hence the findings must be interpreted with caution since they cannot be directly generalized to children and older adults. The samples in the experiments were largely female. Further experiments

targeting other groups (i.e., older adults, children) would help one to understand the extent to which the present findings could be replicated.

The behavioural tasks used for measuring executive functions have limitations as well. Low arousal affective stimuli were used in the affective versions of the tasks. It is unclear whether high-arousal affective stimuli could produce a similar pattern of results. Hence it would be interesting to see a future study using highly arousing stimuli in the tasks measuring the executive functions.

Although the implementation of emotion regulation strategies can be implicit or explicit, only the latter was deployed throughout the research. The use of explicit emotion regulation in the laboratory, despite its inherent benefits, could be influenced by demand characteristics. The advantage of explicit emotion regulation strategy in the laboratory is that the experimenter is able to record the timepoint at which a strategy is initiated and disengaged. This also helps to observe the immediate outcome of the implementation of the emotion regulation strategy. However, the adoption of the explicit emotion regulation strategy could result in some participants merely following the instructions and responding in the way that would appear desirable. Implicit emotion regulation, which occurs naturally and unconsciously, could help explain how executive functions serve as a cognitive basis of emotion regulation. It would therefore be recommended that future research considers using implicit emotion regulation strategies in the laboratory.

Moving forward, future experiments could address these limitations and employ experimental designs that would help to shed more light on the relationship between executive functions and emotion regulation. This would help expand this interesting and highly relevant topic in improving individuals' wellbeing.

Список литературы диссертационного исследования кандидат наук Мохаммед Абдул-Рахеем, 2022 год

References

1. Aker, M., Harmer, C., & Landro, N. I. (2014). More rumination and less effective emotion regulation in previously depressed women with preserved executive functions. BMC Psychiatry, 14(1): https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-014-0334-4

2. Aker, M., & Landro, N. I. (2014). Executive control of emotional processing: A set-shifting task. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 28(8), 1311-1320. https://doi.org/10.1080/13854046.2014.984762

3. Aldao, A., & Dixon-Gordon, K. L. (2014). Broadening the scope of research on emotion regulation strategies and psychopathology. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 43(1), 22-33. https://doi.org/10.1080/16506073.2013.816769

4. Aldao, A. (2013). The future of emotion regulation research: Capturing context. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(2), 155-172. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691612459518

5. Aldao, A., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2012). The influence of context on the implementation of adaptive emotion regulation strategies. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 50(7-8), 493-501. https://doi.org/10.1016Zj.brat.2012.04.004

6. Aldao, A., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Schweizer, S. (2010). Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 30(2), 217-237. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.004

7. Alvarez, J. A., & Emory, E. (2006). Executive function and the frontal lobes: A meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology Review, 16(1), 17-42. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-006-9002-x

8. Andrés, P., & Van der Linden, M. (2000). Age-related differences in supervisory attentional system functions. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 55(6), P373-P380. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/55.6P373

9. Appleton, A. A., Buka, S. L., Loucks, E. B., Gilman, S. E., & Kubzansky, L. D. (2013). Divergent associations of adaptive and maladaptive emotion regulation

99

strategies with inflammation. Health Psychology, 32(7), 748756. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030068

10. Appleton, A. A., Loucks, E. B., Buka, S. L., & Kubzansky, L. D. (2014). Divergent associations of antecedent and response-focused emotion regulation strategies with midlife cardiovascular disease risk. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 48, 246-255. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12160-014-9600-4

11. Azbel-Jackson, L., Butler, L. T., Ellis, J. A., & Van Reekum, C. M. (2016). Stay calm! Regulating emotional responses by implementation intentions: Assessing the impact on physiological and subjective arousal. Cognition and Emotion, 30(6), 1107-1121. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2015.1049515

12. Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory: Looking back and looking forward. Nature ReviewsNeuroscience, 4(10), 829-839. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1201

13. Baddeley, A. (2010). Working memory. Current Biology, 20(4), R136-R140. https://doi.org/10.1016/jxub.2009.12.014

14. Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. Psychology of Learning and Motivation 8, 47-89. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-7421(08)60452-1

15. Badre, D., & Wagner, A. D. (2006). Computational and neurobiological mechanisms underlying cognitive flexibility. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 703(18), 7186-7191. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0509550103

16. Bailey, C. E. (2007). Cognitive accuracy and intelligent executive function in the brain and in business. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1118(1), 122-141. https://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1412.011

17. Barkley, R. A. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin, 121(1), 65-94. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.121.L65

18. Barkley, R. A. (1999). Response inhibition in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Mental retardation and developmental disabilities research reviews, 5(3),

177-184. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1098-2779(1999)5:3<177::AID-

MRDD3>3.0.C0;2-G

19. Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal statistical society: series B, 57(1), 289-300. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2517-6161.1995.tb02031.x

20. Benvenuti, S. M., Buodo, G., Dal Bo, E., & Palomba, D. (2020). Attention and affect in dysphoria: Insights from startle reflex modulation and cardiac deceleration. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 131, 103626. https://doi.org/10.1016/j .brat.2020.103626

21. Berking M., & Whitley B. (2014) The Adaptive Coping with Emotions Model (ACE Model). In Affect regulation training. A practitioner's manual (pp. 19-29). New York, NY: Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4939-1022-9_3

22. Besharat, M. A., Nia, M. E., & Farahani, H. (2013). Anger and major depressive disorder: The mediating role of emotion regulation and anger rumination. Asian Journal of Psychiatry, 6(1), 35-41. https://doi.org/10.1016Zj.ajp.2012.07.013

23. Bishop, S. R., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Carlson, L., Anderson, N. D., Carmody, J., Segal, Z. V., Abbey, S., Speca, M., Velting, D., & Devins, G. (2004). Mindfulness: A proposed operational definition. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 11(3), 230-241. https://doi.org/10.1093/clipsy.bph077

24. Blair, C., & Razza, R. P. (2007). Relating effortful control, executive function, and false belief understanding to emerging math and literacy ability in kindergarten. Child Development, 78(2), 647-663. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01019.x

25. Blumstein, D. T. (2016). Habituation and sensitization: New thoughts about old ideas. Animal Behaviour, 120, 255-262. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2016.05.012

26. Borkovec, T. D., Alcaine, O., & Behar, E. (2004). Avoidance theory of worry and generalized anxiety disorder. Generalized anxiety disorder. In R. G. Heimberg, C.

L. Turk, & D. S. Mennin (Eds.), Advances in Research and Practice (pp. 77108). New York: Guilford Press.

27. Boucsein, W. (2012). Electrodermal activity. Springer Science & Business Media.

28. Bradley, M. M., Codispoti, M., Cuthbert, B. N., & Lang, P. J. (2001). Emotion and motivation I: Defensive and appetitive reactions in picture processing. Emotion, 1(3), 276-298. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.L3.276

29. Bradley, M. M., Keil, A., & Lang, P. J. (2012). Orienting and emotional perception: facilitation, attenuation, and interference. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 493. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00493

30. Bradley, M.M. & Lang, P.J. (2000). Affective reactions to acoustic stimuli. Psychophysiology, 37 (2), 204-215. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-8986.3720204

31. Braunstein, L. M., Gross, J. J., & Ochsner, K. N. (2017). Explicit and implicit emotion regulation: A multi-level framework. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12, 1545-1557. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/ scan/nsx096

32. Bulut, N. S., Würz, A., Küpeli, N. Y., Bulut, G. £., & Sungur, M. Z. (2018). Heart rate variability response to affective pictures processed in and outside of conscious awareness: Three consecutive studies on emotional regulation. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 129, 18-30. https://doi.org/10.1016/jijpsycho.2018.05.006

33. Burton, K. W. (2011). Habitual emotion regulation and the facial grimace. Psychological Reports, 109(2), 521-532. https://doi.org/10.2466/07.09.21.PR0.109.5.521-532

34. Cai, R. Y., Richdale, A. L., Dissanayake, C., Trollor, J., & Uljarevic, M. (2019). Emotion regulation in autism: Reappraisal and suppression interactions. Autism, 23(3), 737-749. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361318774558

35. Carlson, J. M., & Mujica-Parodi, L. R. (2010). A disposition to reappraise decreases anterior insula reactivity during anxious anticipation. Biological Psychology, 85(3), 383-385. https://doi.org/10.1016/j .biopsycho.2010.08.010

36. Carriedo, N., Corral, A., Montoro, P. R., Herrero, L., & Rucian, M. (2016). Development of the updating executive function: From 7-year-olds to young adults. Developmental Psychology, 52(4), 666-678. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000091

37. Chechko, N., Wehrle, R., Erhardt, A., Holsboer, F., Czisch, M., & Sämann, P. G. (2009). Unstable prefrontal response to emotional conflict and activation of lower limbic structures and brainstem in remitted panic disorder. PloS ONE, 4(5): e5537. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0005537

38. Cliff, N. (1993). Dominance statistics: Ordinal analyses to answer ordinal questions. Psychological Bulletin, 114(3), 494-509. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.114.3.494

39. Cludius, B., Mennin, D., & Ehring, T. (2020). Emotion regulation as a transdiagnostic process. Emotion, 20, 37-42. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/emo0000646

40. Cohen, N., & Mor, N. (2018). Enhancing reappraisal by linking cognitive control and emotion. Clinical Psychological Science, 6(1), 155-163. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702617731379

41. Conover, W.J. (1998). Practical Nonparametric Statistics, vol. 350. John Wiley & Sons.

42. Cools R. (2015). Neuropsychopharmacology of cognitive flexibility. In: Toga AW, (Ed). Brain Mapping an Encyclopedic Reference. Waltham (MA): Academic Press. p. 349-353.

43. Cowan, N., Elliott, E. M., Saults, J. S., Morey, C. C., Mattox, S., Hismjatullina, A., & Conway, A. R. (2005). On the capacity of attention: Its estimation and its role in working memory and cognitive aptitudes. Cognitive Psychology, 51(1), 42-100. https://doi.org/10.1016/jxogpsych.2004.12.001

44. Cristofori, I., Cohen-Zimerman, S., & Grafman, J. (2019). Executive functions. In Handbook of Clinical Neurology 163, (pp. 197-219). https://doi.org/10.1016/ B978-0-12-804281-6.00011-2

45. Curci, A., Lanciano, T., Soleti, E., & Rimé, B. (2013). Negative emotional experiences arouse rumination and affect working memory capacity. Emotion, 13(5), 867. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032492

46. D'esposito, M., Detre, J. A., Alsop, D. C., Shin, R. K., Atlas, S., & Grossman, M. (1995). The neural basis of the central executive system of working memory. Nature, 378(6554), 279-281. https://doi.org/10.1038/378279a0

47. De Castella, K., Platow, M. J., Tamir, M., & Gross, J. J. (2018). Beliefs about emotion: Implications for avoidance-based emotion regulation and psychological health. Cognition and Emotion, 32(4), 773-795. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2017.1353485

48. Daucourt, M. C., Schatschneider, C., Connor, C. M., Al Otaiba, S., & Hart, S. A. (2018). Inhibition, updating working memory, and shifting predict reading disability symptoms in a hybrid model: Project KIDS. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 238. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00238

49. Davis, J. C., Marra, C. A., Najafzadeh, M., & Liu-Ambrose, T. (2010). The independent contribution of executive functions to health related quality of life in older women. BMC geriatrics, 10:10. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2318-10-16

50. De Lissnyder, E., Koster, E. H., Derakshan, N., & De Raedt, R. (2010). The association between depressive symptoms and executive control impairments in response to emotional and non-emotional information. Cognition and Emotion, 24(2), 264-280. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930903378354

51. De Pascalis, V., Barry, R. J., & Sparita, A. (1995). Decelerative changes in heart rate during recognition of visual stimuli: Effects of psychological stress. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 20(1), 21-31. https://doi.org/10.1016/0167-8760(95)00023-L

52. Deák, G. O., & Narasimham, G. (2003). Is perseveration caused by inhibition failure? Evidence from preschool children's inferences about word meanings. Journal of

Experimental Child Psychology, 86(3), 194-222.

https://doi.org/10.1016/jjecp.2003.08.001

53. Denny, B. T., & Ochsner, K. N. (2014). Behavioral effects of longitudinal training in cognitive reappraisal. Emotion, 14(2), 425-433. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035276

54. Denson, T. F., Pedersen, W. C., Friese, M., Hahm, A., & Roberts, L. (2011). Understanding impulsive aggression: Angry rumination and reduced self-control capacity are mechanisms underlying the provocation-aggression relationship. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37(6), 850-862. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167211401420

55. Deveney, C. M., & Deldin, P. J. (2006). A preliminary investigation of cognitive flexibility for emotional information in major depressive disorder and non-psychiatric controls. Emotion, 6(3), 429. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.6.3.429

56. Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135168. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750

57. Dimberg, U. & Karlsson, B. (1997). Facial reactions to different emotionally relevant stimuli. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 38, 297-303. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9450.00039.

58. Dovis, S., Van der Oord, S., Wiers, R. W., & Prins, P. J. (2013). What part of working memory is not working in ADHD? Short-term memory, the central executive and effects of reinforcement. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 41(6), 901-917. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-013-9729-9

59. Dreisbach, G., & Goschke, T. (2004). How positive affect modulates cognitive control: Reduced perseveration at the cost of increased distractibility. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30(2), 343-353. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.30.2.343

60. Driscoll, D., Tranel, D., & Anderson, S. W. (2009). The effects of voluntary regulation of positive and negative emotion on psychophysiological

responsiveness. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 72(1), 61-66. https://doi.org/10.1016/jijpsycho.2008.03.012

61. Dvorak, R. D., Sargent, E. M., Kilwein, T. M., Stevenson, B. L., Kuvaas, N. J., & Williams, T. J. (2014). Alcohol use and alcohol-related consequences: Associations with emotion regulation difficulties. The American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse, 40(2), 125-130. https://doi.org/10.3109/00952990.2013.877920

62. Efinger, L., Thuillard, S., & Dan-Glauser, E. S. (2019). Distraction and reappraisal efficiency on immediate negative emotional responses: Role of trait anxiety. Anxiety, Stress and Coping, 32(4), 412-427. https://doi.org/10.1080/10615806.2019.1597859

63. Ehring, T., Tuschen-Caffier, B., Schnulle, J., Fischer, S., & Gross, J. J. (2010). Emotion regulation and vulnerability to depression: spontaneous versus instructed use of emotion suppression and reappraisal. Emotion, 10(4), 563. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019010

64. English, T., John, O. P., Srivastava, S., & Gross, J. J. (2012). Emotion regulation and peer-rated social functioning: A 4-year longitudinal study. Journal of Research in Personality, 46, 780-784. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/jjrp.2012.09.006

65. Erber, R., Wegner, D. M., & Therriault, N. (1996). On being cool and collected: Mood regulation in anticipation of social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70(4), 757. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.70A757

66. Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception & Psychophysics, 16(1), 143-149. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03203267

67. Fairchild, G., van Goozen, S. H., Stollery, S. J., Aitken, M. R., Savage, J., Moore, S. C., & Goodyer, I. M. (2009). Decision making and executive function in male adolescents with early-onset or adolescence-onset conduct disorder and control subjects. Biological Psychiatry, 66(2), 162-168. https://doi.org/10.1016/j .biopsych.2009.02.024

68. Feifel, D., Farber, R. H., Clementz, B. A., Perry, W., & Anllo-Vento, L. (2004). Inhibitory deficits in ocular motor behavior in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological psychiatry, 56(5), 333-339. https://doi.org/10.10167j.biopsych.2004.06.019

69. Fink, G. R., Marshall, J. C., Halligan, P. W., Frith, C. D., Frackowiak, R. S., & Dolan, R. J. (1997). Hemispheric specialization for global and local processing: The effect of stimulus category. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 264(1381): 487-494. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.1997.0070

70. Fischer, A. H., Manstead, A. S. R., Evers, C., Timmers, M., & Valk, G.(2004). Motives and norms underlying emotion regulation. In P. Philip-pot & R. S. Feldman (Eds.), The regulation of emotion (pp. 187-210). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

71. Friedman, N. P., & Miyake, A. (2004). The relations among inhibition and interference control functions: A latent-variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133(1), 101-135. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.133.L101

72. Friedman, N. P., & Miyake, A. (2017). Unity and diversity of executive functions: Individual differences as a window on cognitive structure. Cortex, 86, 186-204. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2016.04.023

73. Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Corley, R. P., Young, S. E., DeFries, J. C., & Hewitt, J. K. (2006). Not all executive functions are related to intelligence. Psychological Science, 17(2), 172-179. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01681.x

74. Fuentes-Sánchez, N., Jaén, I., Escrig, M. A., Lucas, I., Pastor, M. C. (2019). Cognitive reappraisal during unpleasant picture processing: Subjective self-report and peripheral physiology. Psychophysiology, 56(8): e13372. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.13372

75. Gajewski, P. D., Hanisch, E., Falkenstein, M., Thones, S., & Wascher, E. (2018). What does the n-back task measure as we get older? Relations between working-memory measures and other cognitive functions across the lifespan. Frontiers in Psychology, 9: 2208. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02208

76. Gärtner, A., Jawinski, P., & Strobel, A. (2021, January 21). Individual differences in inhibitory control are not related to emotion regulation. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/cd8rx

77. Giles, G. E., Cantelon, J. A., Eddy, M. D., Brunyé, T. T., Urry, H. L., Taylor, H. A., ... & Kanarek, R. B. (2018). Cognitive reappraisal reduces perceived exertion during endurance exercise. Motivation and Emotion, 42(4), 482-496. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-018-9697-z

78. Gläscher, J., Adolphs, R., & Tranel, D. (2019). Model-based lesion mapping of cognitive control using the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Nature Communications, 10(1), 1-12. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-018-07912-5

79. Goeleven, E., De Raedt, R., Baert, S., & Koster, E. H. (2006). Deficient inhibition of emotional information in depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 93, 149-157. https://doi.org/10.1016/jjad.2006.03.007

80. Goldstein, B., Obrzut, J. E., John, C., Ledakis, G., & Armstrong, C. L. (2004). The impact of frontal and non-frontal brain tumor lesions on Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance. Brain and Cognition, 54(2), 110-116. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00269-0

81. Gomez, P., Scholz, U., & Danuser, B. (2015). The down-regulation of disgust by implementation intentions: experiential and physiological concomitants. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 40(2), 95-106. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10484-015-9280-2

82. Gordan, R., Gwathmey, J. K., & Xie, L. H. (2015). Autonomic and endocrine control of cardiovascular function. World Journal of Cardiology, 7(4), 204. https://doi.org/10.4330/wjc.v7.i4.204

83. Grandey, A. A. (2000). Emotional regulation in the workplace: A new way to conceptualize emotional labor. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 5(1), 95110. https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-8998.5.1.95

84. Grant, D. A., & Berg, E. (1948). A behavioral analysis of degree of reinforcement and ease of shifting to new responses in a Weigl-type card-sorting problem. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38(4), 404-411. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0059831

85. Gratz, K. L., & Roemer, L. (2004). Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation: Development, factor structure, and initial validation of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 26(1), 41-54. https://doi.org/10.1023/BJ0BA.0000007455.08539.94

86. Gross, J. J. (1998). Antecedent-and response-focused emotion regulation: Divergent consequences for experience, expression, and physiology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(1), 224. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.74.L224

87. Gross, J. J. (2002). Emotion regulation: Affective, cognitive, and social consequences. Psychophysiology, 39(3), 281-291. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0048577201393198

88. Gross, J. J. (2014). Emotion regulation: Conceptual and empirical foundations. In J. J. Grossman (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 3-20), 2nd ed. New York: The Guilford Press.

89. Gross, J. J. (2015). Emotion Regulation: Current status and future prospects. Psychological Inquiry, 26, 1-26. https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2014.940781

90. Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(2), 348-362. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348

91. Gross, J. J., & Levenson, R. W. (1997). Hiding feelings: The acute effects of inhibiting negative and positive emotion. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106(1), 95-103. https://doi.org/10.1037/Z0021-843x.106.L95

92. Gyurak, A., Goodkind, M. S., Kramer, J. H., Miller, B. L., & Levenson, R. W. (2012). Executive functions and the down-regulation and up-regulation of emotion. Cognition & emotion, 26(1), 103-118. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2011.557291

93. Harfmann, E. J., Rhyner, K. T., & Ingram, R. E. (2019). Cognitive inhibition and attentional biases in the affective go/no-go performance of depressed, suicidal populations. Journal of Affective Disorders, 256, 228-233. https://doi.org/10.1016/jjad.2019.05.022

94. Harnett, N. G., Wood, K. H., Wheelock, M. D., Knight, A. J., & Knight, D. C. (2017). Anticipation and the neural response to threat. In Anticipation and Medicine (pp. 219228). Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-45142-8_13

95. Harvey, P. O., Le Bastard, G., Pochon, J. B., Levy, R., Allilaire, J. F., Dubois, B. E. E. A., & Fossati, P. (2004). Executive functions and updating of the contents of working memory in unipolar depression. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 38(6), 567576. https://doi.org/10.1016/j .jpsychires.2004.03.003

96. Harwood, K., Wickerts, C., Kramer, A., Clay, D., & Liu, Y. (1986). Effects of display proximity and memory demands on the understanding of dynamic multidimensional information. Proceedings of the Human Factors Society Annual Meeting, 30(8), 786789. https://doi.org/10.1177/154193128603000812

97. Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K., & Wilson, K. (1999). Acceptance and commitment therapy: An experiential approach to behavior change. New York: Guilford Press.

98. Hayes, S. C., Wilson, K. G., Gifford, E. V., Follette, V. M., & Strosahl, K. (1996). Experiential avoidance and behavioral disorders: A functional dimensional approach to diagnosis and treatment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 11521168. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.64.6.1152

99. Heilman, R. M., Cri§an, L. G., Houser, D., Miclea, M., & Miu, A. C. (2010). Emotion regulation and decision making under risk and uncertainty. Emotion, 10(2), 257265. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018489

100. Hendricks, M. A., & Buchanan, T. W. (2016). Individual differences in cognitive control processes and their relationship to emotion regulation. Cognition and Emotion, 30(5), 912-924. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2015.1032893

101. Hofmann, S. G., Carpenter, J. K., & Curtiss, J. (2016). Interpersonal emotion regulation questionnaire (IERQ): Scale development and psychometric characteristics. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 40(3), 341-356. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-016-9756-2

102. Hommel, B. (2011). The Simon effect as tool and heuristic. Acta Psychologica, 136(2), 189-202. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.04.011

103. Hommel, B., Ridderinkhof, R. K., & Theeuwes, J. (2002). Cognitive control of attention and action: Issues and trends. Psychological Research, 66(4), 215-219. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-002-0096-3

104. Hoorelbeke, K., Koster, E. H. W., Demeyer, I., Loeys, T., & Vanderhasselt, M.-A. (2016). Effects of cognitive control training on the dynamics of (mal)adaptive emotion regulation in daily life. Emotion, 16(7), 945956. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000169

105. Hubner, R., & Tobel, L. (2019). Conflict resolution in the Eriksen flanker task: Similarities and differences to the Simon task. PloS ONE, 14(3): e0214203. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0214203

106. Ivcevic, Z., & Brackett, M. (2014). Predicting school success: Comparing conscientiousness, grit, and emotion regulation ability. Journal of Research in Personality, 52, 29-36. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/jjrp.2014.06 .005

107. IBM Corp. Released 2012. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 21.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp.

108. Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., Perrig, W. J., & Meier, B. (2010). The concurrent validity of the N-back task as a working memory measure. Memory, 18(4), 394-412. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211003702171

109. JASP Team (2020). JASP (Version 0.14.1)[Computer software].

110. Jensen, A. R., & Rohwer, W. D. (1966). The Stroop word-color test: a review. Acta Psychologies, 25, 36, 93.

111. Jersild, A. T. (1927). Mental set and shift. Archives of Psychology, 14, 89, 81.

112. Joormann, J. (2004). Attentional bias in dysphoria: The role of inhibitory processes. Cognition and Emotion, 18(1), 125-147. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930244000480

113. Joormann, J., & Stanton, C. H. (2016). Examining emotion regulation in depression: A review and future directions. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 86, 35-49. https://doi.org/10.1016/) .brat.2016.07.007

114. Joormann, J., Yoon, K. L., & Zetsche, U. (2007). Cognitive inhibition in depression. Applied and Preventive Psychology, 12(3), 128-139. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appsy.2007.09.002

115. Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W. (2003). Working-memory capacity and the control of attention: the contributions of goal neglect, response competition, and task set to Stroop interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132(1), 47. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.132.L47

116. Kelley, N. J., Glazer, J. E., Pornpattananangkul, N., & Nusslock, R. (2019). Reappraisal and suppression emotion-regulation tendencies differentially predict reward-responsivity and psychological well-being. Biological Psychology, 140, 3547. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2018.11.005

117. Kimchi, R., & Palmer, S. E. (1982). Form and texture in hierarchically constructed patterns. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 8(4), 521-535. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.8A521

118. Kinner, V. L., Kuchinke, L., Dierolf, A. M., Merz, C. J., Otto, T., & Wolf, O. T. (2017). What our eyes tell us about feelings: Tracking pupillary responses during emotion regulation processes. Psychophysiology, 54(4), 508-518. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12816

119. Kirchner, W. K. (1958). Age differences in short-term retention of rapidly changing information. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55(4), 352358. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0043688

120. Koole, S. L. (2009). The psychology of emotion regulation: An integrative review. Cognition and Emotion, 23(1), 4-41. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930802619031

121. Koole, S. L., Webb, T. L., & Sheeran, P. L. (2015). Implicit emotion regulation: Feeling better without knowing why. Current Opinion in Psychology, 3, 6-10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2014.12.027

122. Knepp, M. M., Krafka, E. R., & Druzina, E. M. (2015). The impact of trait worry and emotion regulation on heart rate variability. Cogent Psychology, 2(1), 1038896. https://doi.org/10.1080/23311908.2015.1038896

123. Kosonogov, V., Sanchez-Navarro, J. P., Martinez-Selva, J. M., Torrente, G., & Carrillo-Verdejo, E. (2016). Social stimuli increase physiological reactivity but not defensive responses. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 57(5), 393-398. https://doi.org/10.1111/sjop.12311

124. Labuschagne, I., Pedder, D. J., Henry, J. D., Terrett, G., & Rendell, P. G. (2020). Age differences in emotion regulation and facial muscle reactivity to emotional films. Gerontology, 66(1), 74-84. https://doi.org/10.1159/000501584

125. Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1997). International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Technical manual and affective ratings. NIMH Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention (pp. 39-58). Gainesville. https://doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803/a000147 212

126. Lang, P. J., Greenwald, M. K., Bradley, M. M. & Hamm, A. O. (1993). Looking at pictures: Affective, facial, visceral, and behavioral reactions. Psychophysiology, 30, 261-273. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1993.tb03352.x

127. Larsen, R. J. (2000). Toward a science of mood regulation. Psychological Inquiry, 11(3), 129-141. https//doi.org/10.1207/S15327965PLI1103_01

128. Law, K. C., Khazem, L. R., & Anestis, M. D. (2015). The role of emotion dysregulation in suicide as considered through the ideation to action framework. Current Opinion in Psychology, 3, 30-35. https://doi.org/10.1016/jxopsyc.2015.01.014

129. Lee, S. J., Lee, H.-K., Kweon, Y.-S., Lee, C. T., & Lee, K.-U. (2009).

The impact of executive function on emotion recognition and emotion experience in

113

patients with schizophrenia. Psychiatry Investigation, 6(3), 156. https//doi.org/10.4306/pi.2009.6.3.156

130. Levens, S. M., & Gotlib, I. H. (2010). Updating positive and negative stimuli in working memory in depression. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 139(4), 654664. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020283.

131. Levens, S. M., & Gotlib, I. H. (2015). Updating emotional content in recovered depressed individuals: Evaluating deficits in emotion processing following a depressive episode. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 48, 156-163. https://doi.org/10.1016/jjbtep.2015.03.009

132. Lewis, E. J., Yoon, K. L., & Joormann, J. (2018). Emotion regulation and biological stress responding: associations with worry, rumination, and reappraisal. Cognition and Emotion, 32(7), 1487-1498. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2017.1310088

133. Liang, Y., Huo, M., Kennison, R., & Zhou, R. (2017). The role of cognitive control in older adult cognitive reappraisal: Detached and positive reappraisal. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 11, 27. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00027

134. Liberman, N., Sagristano, M. D., & Trope, Y. (2002). The effect of temporal distance on level of mental construal. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38(6), 523534. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-1031(02)00535-8

135. Livingstone, K. M., & Isaacowitz, D. M. (2018). The roles of age and attention in general emotion regulation, reappraisal, and expressive suppression. Psychology and Aging, 33(3), 373. https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000240

136. Lohani, M., & Isaacowitz, D. M. (2014). Age differences in managing response to sadness elicitors using attentional deployment, positive reappraisal and suppression. Cognition & Emotion, 28(4), 678-697. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2013.853648

137. Lui, M., & Tannock, R. (2007). Working memory and inattentive behaviour in a community sample of children. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 3(1), 1-11. https://doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-3-12

138. Makowski, D., Sperduti, M., Pelletier, J., Blonde, P., La Corte, V., Arcangeli, M., ... & Piolino, P. (2019). Phenomenal, bodily and brain correlates of fictional reappraisal as an implicit emotion regulation strategy. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 19(4), 877-897. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-018-00681-0

139. Malooly, A. M., Genet, J. J., & Siemer, M. (2013). Individual differences in reappraisal effectiveness: The role of affective flexibility. Emotion, 13(2), 302-313. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029980

140. Matsuo, K., Glahn, D. C., Peluso, M. A. M., Hatch, J. P., Monkul, E. S., Najt, P., ... & Soares, J. C. (2007). Prefrontal hyperactivation during working memory task in untreated individuals with major depressive disorder. Molecular Psychiatry, 12(2), 158-166. https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.mp.4001894

141. Mayr, U., & Kliegl, R. (2000). Task-set switching and long-term memory retrieval [Editorial]. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26(5), 1124-1140. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.26.5.1124

142. McRae, K., & Gross, J. J. (2020). Emotion regulation. Emotion, 20(1), 1-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/emo0000703

143. McRae, K., Jacobs, S. E., Ray, R. D., John, O. P., & Gross, J. J. (2012). Individual differences in reappraisal ability: Links to reappraisal frequency, well-being, and cognitive control. Journal of Research in Personality, 46(1), 2-7. https://doi.org/10.1016/jjrp.2011.10.003

144. Mennin, D., & Farach, F. (2007). Emotion and evolving treatments for adult psychopathology. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 14(4), 329352. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2850.2007.00094.x

145. Miles, S., Howlett, C. A., Berryman, C., Nedeljkovic, M., Moseley, G. L., & Phillipou, A. (2021). Considerations for using the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test to assess cognitive flexibility. Behavior Research Methods, 53, 2083-2091. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-021-01551-3

146. Milner, B. (1964). Some effects of frontal lobectomy in man. The Frontal Granular Cortex and Behavior., 313-334.

147. Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex "frontal lobe" tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49-100. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1999.0734

148. Morris, N., & Jones, D. M. (1990). Memory updating in working memory: The role of the central executive. British journal of psychology, 81(2), 111-121. https://doi.org/10.1111/j .2044-8295.1990.tb02349.x

149. Mukhopadhyay, P., Dutt, A., Das, S. K., Basu, A., Hazra, A., Dhibar, T., & Roy, T. (2007). Identification of neuroanatomical substrates of set-shifting ability: evidence from patients with focal brain lesions. Progress in Brain Research, 168, 95-104. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-6123(07)68008-X

150. Mukunda, K. V., & Hall, V. C. (1992). Does performance on memory for order correlate with performance on standardized measures of ability? A meta-analysis. Intelligence, 16(1), 81-97. https://doi.org/10.1016/0160-2896(92)90026-N

151. Naragon-Gainey, K., McMahon, T. P., & Chacko, T. P. (2017). The structure of common emotion regulation strategies: A meta-analytic examination. Psychological Bulletin, 143(4), 384-427. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000093

152. Nasso, S., Vanderhasselt, M.-A., Demeyer, I., & De Raedt, R. (2019). Autonomic regulation in response to stress: The influence of anticipatory emotion regulation strategies and trait rumination. Emotion, 19(3), 443-454. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000448

153. Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception. Cognitive Psychology, 9(3), 353-383. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(77)90012-3

154. Navon, D. (1981). The forest revisited: More on global precedence. Psychological Research, 43(1), 1-32. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00309635

155. Neudert, M. K., Stark, R., Kress, L., & Hermann, A. (2017). Trait worry and neural correlates of emotion regulation. Zeitschrift für Psychologie / Journal of Psychology, 225(3): https://doi.org/10.1027/2151 -2604/a000305

156. Niedenthal, P. M., & Ric, F. (2017). Psychology of emotion. (2nd ed.) Psychology Press. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315276229

157. Niendam, T. A., Laird, A. R., Ray, K. L., Dean, Y. M., Glahn, D. C., & Carter, C. S. (2012). Meta-analytic evidence for a superordinate cognitive control network subserving diverse executive functions. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 72, 241-268. http://dx.doi.org/10 .3758/s13415-011-0083-5

158. Nittel, C. M., Lincoln, T. M., Lamster, F., Leube, D., Rief, W., Kircher, T., & Mehl, S. (2018). Expressive suppression is associated with state paranoia in psychosis: An experience sampling study on the association between adaptive and maladaptive emotion regulation strategies and paranoia. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 57(3), 291-312. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjc.12174

159. Niven, K. (2017). The four key characteristics of interpersonal emotion regulation. Current Opinion in Psychology, 17, 89-93. https://doi.org/10.1016/jxopsyc.2017.06.015

160. Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Morrow, J., & Fredrickson, B. L. (1993). Response styles and the duration of episodes of depressed mood. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 20-28. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.102.L20

161. Norman, D. A., & Shallice, T. (1980). Attention to action: Willed and automatic control of behavior technical report No. 8006. Center for Human Information Processing, University of California, San Diego.

162. O'Reilly, H., Lundqvist, D., Pigat, D., Baron, K., Fridenson, S., Tal, S., Meir, N., Berggren, S., Lassalle, A., Golan, O., Bolte, S., Piana, S., Rotman, C., Coletta, P., Marchi, E., Davies, I., Sullings, N., Baranger, A., Gauvain, C., Schuller, B., Newman, S., Camurri, A., Robinson, P. & Baron-Cohen, S. (2012). The EU-Emotion Stimulus Set, Cambridge, UK: Autism Research Centre, University of Cambridge.

163. O'Reilly, H., Pigat, D., Fridenson, S., Berggren, S., Tal, S., Golan, O., Bolte, S., Baron-Cohen, S. & Lundqvist, D. (2015). The EU-emotion stimulus set: A validation study. Behavior Research Methods. 48(2), 567-576. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-015-0601-4

164. Ossenfort, K. L., Harris, J. A., Platzek, C., & Isaacowitz, D. M. (2019). Positive and detached reappraisal in older adulthood: A temporal examination of gaze patterns. Aging & mental health, 23(12), 1661-1665. https://doi.org/10.1080/13607863.2018.1506752

165. Pankratova, A.A., & Kornienko, D. S. (2017). A Russian adaptation of the Emotional Regulation Questionnaire. Voprosy Psikhologii, 139-149.

166. Parkinson, B., & Totterdell, P. (1999). Classifying affect-regulation strategies. Cognition & Emotion, 13(3), 277-303. https://doi.org/10.1080/026999399379285

167. Pavlov, I. P. (1927). Conditioned reflexes: An investigation of the physiological activity of the cerebral cortex.

168. Pe, M. L., Koval, P., Houben, M., Erbas, Y., Champagne, D., & Kuppens, P. (2015). Updating in working memory predicts greater emotion reactivity to and facilitated recovery from negative emotion-eliciting stimuli. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 372. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00372

169. Pe, M. L., Koval, P., & Kuppens, P. (2013a). Executive well-being: Updating of positive stimuli in working memory is associated with subjective well-being. Cognition, 126(2), 335-340. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.10.002

170. Pe, M. L., Raes, F., & Kuppens, P. (2013b). The cognitive building blocks of emotion regulation: Ability to update working memory moderates the efficacy of rumination and reappraisal on emotion. PLOS ONE, 8(7): e69071. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0069071

171. Peirce, J., Gray, J. R., Simpson, S., MacAskill, M., Hochenberger, R., Sogo, H., ... & Lindelov, J. K. (2019). PsychoPy2: Experiments in behavior made easy. Behavior Research Methods, 51(1), 195-203. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-018-01193-y

172. Pessoa, L. (2009). How do emotion and motivation direct executive control? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(4), 160-166. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.01.006

173. Pessoa, L., Padmala, S., Kenzer, A., & Bauer, A. (2012). Interactions between cognition and emotion during response inhibition. Emotion, 12(1), 192197. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024109

174. Peters, B. J., & Jamieson, J. P. (2016). The consequences of suppressing affective displays in romantic relationships: A challenge and threat perspective. Emotion, 16(7), 1050-1066. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000202

175. Petit, G., Luminet, O., Maurage, F., Tecco, J., Lechantre, S., Ferauge, M., ... & de Timary, P. (2015). Emotion regulation in alcohol dependence. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 39(12), 2471-2479. https://doi.org/10.1111/acer.12914

176. Poljac, E., Simon, S., Ringlever, L., Kalcik, D., Groen, W. B., Buitelaar, J. K., & Bekkering, H. (2010). Impaired task switching performance in children with dyslexia but not in children with autism. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63(2), 401-416. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210902990803

177. Pollack, I., Johnson, L. B., & Knaff, P. R. (1959). Running memory span. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57(3), 137-146. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0046137

178. Postle, B. R. (2003). Context in verbal short-term memory. Memory & Cognition, 31(8), 1198-1207. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03195803

179. Powers, J. P., & LaBar, K. S. (2019). Regulating emotion through distancing: A taxonomy, neurocognitive model, and supporting meta-analysis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 96, 155-173. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.04.023

180. Prevor, M. B., & Diamond, A. (2005). Color-object interference in young children: A Stroop effect in children 3^-6% years old. Cognitive Development, 20(2), 256-278. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2005.04.001

181. Pruessner, L., Barnow, S., Holt, D. V, Joormann, J., & Schulze, K. (2020). A cognitive control framework for understanding emotion regulation flexibility. Emotion, 20(1), 21-29. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000658

182. Puric, D., & Pavlovic, M. (2012). Executive function of shifting: Factorial structure and relations to personality and intelligence domains. Suvremena Psihologija, 15(2), 177-191.

183. Radkovsky, A., McArdle, J. J., Bockting, C. L. H., & Berking, M. (2014). Successful emotion regulation skills application predicts subsequent reduction of symptom severity during treatment of major depressive disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 82(2), 248-262. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035828

184. Ray, R. D., McRae, K., Ochsner, K. N., & Gross, J. J. (2010). Cognitive reappraisal of negative affect: Converging evidence from EMG and self-report. Emotion, 10(4), 587-592. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019015

185. Richards, J. M., & Gross, J. J. (2000). Emotion regulation and memory: The cognitive costs of keeping one's cool. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(3), 410424. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.79.3410

186. Roemer, L., Williston, S. K., & Rollins, L. G. (2015). Mindfulness and emotion regulation. Current Opinion in Psychology, 3, 52-57. https://doi.org/10.1016/jxopsyc.2015.02.006

187. Rogers, R. D., & Monsell, S. (1995). Costs of a predictible switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124(2), 207-231. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.124.2.207

188. Salo, R., Henik, A., & Robertson, L. C. (2001). Interpreting Stroop interference: An analysis of differences between task versions. Neuropsychology, 15(4), 462471. https://doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.15A462

189. Sánchez-Navarro, J. P., Martínez-Selva, J. M., & Román, F. (2006). Uncovering the relationship between defence and orienting in emotion: Cardiac reactivity to unpleasant pictures. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 61(1), 34-46.

190. Scheibe, S., Sheppes, G., & Staudinger, U. M. (2015). Distract or reappraise? Age-related differences in emotion-regulation choice. Emotion, 15(6), 677681. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0039246

191. Schmeichel, B. J., & Tang, D. (2015). Individual differences in executive functioning and their relationship to emotional processes and responses. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24(2), 93-98. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721414555178

192. Schmeichel, B. J., Volokhov, R. N., & Demaree, H. A. (2008). Working memory capacity and the self-regulation of emotional expression and experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(6), 1526-1540. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013345

193. Schweizer, S., Satpute, A. B., Atzil, S., Field, A. P., Hitchcock, C., Black, M., Barrett, L. F., & Dalgleish, T. (2019). The impact of affective information on working memory: A pair of meta-analytic reviews of behavioral and neuroimaging evidence. Psychological Bulletin. 145(6), 566-609. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/bul0000193

194. Shallice, T. (1982). Specific impairments of planning. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. B, Biological Sciences, 298(1089), 199-209. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.1982.0082

195. Sheppes, G., & Meiran, N. (2008). Divergent cognitive costs for online forms of reappraisal and distraction. Emotion, 8(6), 870-874. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013711

196. Shiota, M. N., & Levenson, R. W. (2009). Effects of aging on experimentally instructed detached reappraisal, positive reappraisal, and emotional behavior suppression. Psychology and Aging, 24(4), 890-900. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017896

197. Simon, J. R. (1969). Reactions toward the source of stimulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81(1), 174-176. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0027448

198. Smyth, J. M., Zawadzki, M. J., & Gerin, W. (2013). Stress and disease: A structural and functional analysis. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 7, 217-227. https://doi.org/10.1111/ spc3.12020

199. Sperduti, M., Makowski, D., Arcangeli, M., Wantzen, P., Zalla, T., Lemaire, S., Dokic, J., Pelletier, J., & Piolino, P. (2017). The distinctive role of executive functions

in implicit emotion regulation. Acta Psychologica, 173, 13-20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2016.12.001

200. Stiller, A. K., Kattner, M. F., Gunzenhauser, C., & Schmitz, B. (2019). The effect of positive reappraisal on the availability of self-control resources and self-regulated learning. Educational Psychology, 39(1), 86-111. https://doi.org/10.1080/01443410.2018.1524851

201. Storbeck, J., & Maswood, R. (2016). Happiness increases verbal and spatial working memory capacity where sadness does not: Emotion, working memory and executive control. Cognition and Emotion, 30(5), 925-938. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2015.1034091

202. Strauss, G. P., Ossenfort, K. L., & Whearty, K. M. (2016). Reappraisal and distraction emotion regulation strategies are associated with distinct patterns of visual attention and differing levels of cognitive demand. PloS ONE, 11(11): e0162290. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0162290

203. Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643-662. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0054651

204. Stuss, D. T., Levine, B., Alexander, M. P., Hong, J., Palumbo, C., Hamer, L., ... & Izukawa, D. (2000). Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance in patients with focal frontal and posterior brain damage: effects of lesion location and test structure on separable cognitive processes. Neuropsychologia, 38(4), 388-402. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(99)00093-7

205. Sullivan, S. K., & Strauss, G. P. (2017). Electrophysiological evidence for detrimental impact of a reappraisal emotion regulation strategy on subsequent cognitive control in schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 126(5), 679693. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000285

206. Suri, G., Sheppes, G., & Gross, J. J. (2013). Emotion regulation and cognition. In M. D. Robinson, E. Watkins, & E. Harmon-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of cognition and emotion (pp. 195-209). New York: The Guilford Press.

207. Tamir, M. (2016). Why do people regulate their emotions? A taxonomy of motives in emotion regulation. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 20(3), 199-222. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868315586325

208. Tamir, M., & Ford, B. Q. (2009). Choosing to be afraid: Preferences for fear as a function of goal pursuit. Emotion, 9(4), 488-497. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015882

209. Tamir, M., Mitchell, C., & Gross, J. J. (2008). Hedonic and instrumental motives in anger regulation. Psychological Science, 19(4), 324-328. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02088.x

210. Tang, Y., & Huang, Y. (2019). Contextual factors influence the selection of specific and broad types of emotion regulation strategies. British Journal of Social Psychology, 58(4), 1008-1033. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjso.12313

211. Thiruchselvam, R., Blechert, J., Sheppes, G., Rydstrom, A., & Gross, J. J. (2011). The temporal dynamics of emotion regulation: An EEG study of distraction and reappraisal. Biological psychology, 87(1), 84-92. https://doi.org/10.1016/j .biopsycho.2011.02.009

212. Tull, M. T., & Aldao, A. (2015). Editorial overview: New directions in the science of emotion regulation. Current Opinion in Psychology, 3, iv-x. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.03.009

213. Urry, H. L. (2009). Using reappraisal to regulate unpleasant emotional episodes: Goals and timing matter. Emotion, 9(6), 782-797. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017109

214. Venables, P. H., & Christie, M. J. (1980). Electrodermal activity. Techniques in Psychophysiology, 54(3).

215. Veroude, K., Jolles, J., Croiset, G., & Krabbendam, L. (2013). Changes in neural mechanisms of cognitive control during the transition from late adolescence to young adulthood. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, 63-70. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2012.12.002

216. Verzeletti, C., Zammunaer, V. L., Galli, C., and Agnoli, S. (2016). Emotion regulation strategies and psychosocial well-being in adolescence. Cogent Psychology 3, 1-15. https://doi.org/10.1080/23311908.2016.1199294

217. Visted, E., Vollestad, J., Nielsen, M. B., & Schanche, E. (2018). Emotion regulation in current and remitted depression: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Frontiers in Psychology, 9: 756. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00756

218. Von Hippel, W., & Gonsalkorale, K. (2005). "That is bloody revolting!" Inhibitory control of thoughts better left unsaid. Psychological Science, 16(7), 497-500. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01563.x

219. Ward, G., Roberts, M. J., & Phillips, L. H. (2001). Task-switching costs, Stroop-costs, and executive control: A correlational study. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 54(2), 491-511. https://doi.org/10.1080/713755967

220. Webb, T. L., Miles, E., & Sheeran, P. (2012). Dealing with feeling: A meta-analysis of the effectiveness of strategies derived from the process model of emotion regulation. Psychological Bulletin, 138(4), 775-808. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027600

221. Westwood, H., Stahl, D., Mandy, W., & Tchanturia, K. (2016). The set-shifting profiles of anorexia nervosa and autism spectrum disorder using the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine, 46(9), 1809-1827. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291716000581

222. Whalen, P. J., Bush, G., McNally, R. J., Wilhelm, S., McInerney, S. C., Jenike, M. A., & Rauch, S. L. (1998). The emotional counting Stroop paradigm: A functional magnetic resonance imaging probe of the anterior cingulate affective division. Biological psychiatry, 44(12), 1219-1228. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006-3223(98)00251-0

223. Will Crescioni, A., Ehrlinger, J., Alquist, J. L., Conlon, K. E., Baumeister, R. F., Schatschneider, C., & Dutton, G. R. (2011). High trait self-control predicts positive

health behaviors and success in weight loss. Journal of Health Psychology, 16(5), 750-759. https://doi.org/10.1177/1359105310390247

224. Williams, W. C., Morelli, S. A., Ong, D. C., & Zaki, J. (2018). Interpersonal emotion regulation: Implications for affiliation, perceived support, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 115(2), 224254. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000132

225. Winter, D., Krause-Utz, A., Lis, S., Chiu, C. D., Lanius, R. A., Schriner, F., ... & Schmahl, C. (2015). Dissociation in borderline personality disorder: Disturbed cognitive and emotional inhibition and its neural correlates. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 233(3), 339-351. https://doi.org/10.1016Zj.pscychresns.2015.05.018

226. Witvliet, C. van O., DeYoung, N. J., Hofelich, A. J., & DeYoung, P. A. (2011). Compassionate reappraisal and emotion suppression as alternatives to offense-focused rumination: Implications for forgiveness and psychophysiological well-being. Journal of Positive Psychology, 6(4), 286-299. https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2011.577091

227. Wolgast, M., Lundh, L. G., & Viborg, G. (2011). Cognitive reappraisal and acceptance: An experimental comparison of two emotion regulation strategies. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 49(12), 858-866. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2011.09.011

228. Xie, L., Ren, M., Cao, B., & Li, F. (2017). Distinct brain responses to different inhibitions: Evidence from a modified Flanker task. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 1-10. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-04907-y

229. Xiu, L., Zhou, R., & Jiang, Y. (2016). Working memory training improves emotion regulation ability: Evidence from HRV. Physiology & Behavior, 155, 25-29. https: //doi.org/ 10.1016/j .physbeh.2015.12.004

230. Yeh, N., Barber, S. J., Suri, G., & Opitz, P. (2020). The role of reappraisal success in emotional and memory outcomes. Emotion, 20(6), 939950. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000575

231. Yntema, D. B. (1963). Keeping track of several things at once. Human Factors, 5(1), 7-17. https://doi.org/10.1177/001872086300500102

232. Yuan J, Liu Y, Ding N, Yang, J. (2014). The regulation of induced depression during a frustrating situation: Benefits of expressive suppression in Chinese individuals. PLoS ONE 9(5): e97420. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0097420

233. Zaehringer, J., Jennen-Steinmetz, C., Schmahl, C., Ende, G., & Paret, C. (2020). Psychophysiological effects of downregulating negative emotions: Insights from a meta-analysis of healthy adults. Frontiers in Psychology, 11: 470. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00470

234. Zaki, J., & Williams, W. C. (2013). Interpersonal emotion regulation. Emotion, 13(5), 803-810. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0033839

235. Zamora, E. V., Introzzi, I., Del Valle, M., Vernucci, S., & Richard' s, M. M. (2020). Perceptual inhibition of emotional interference in children. Applied Neuropsychology: Child, 9(3), 215-229. https://doi.org/10.1080/21622965.2019.1567340

236. Zawadzki, M. J. (2015). Rumination is independently associated with poor psychological health: Comparing emotion regulation strategies. Psychology & Health, 30(10), 1146-1163. https://doi.org/10.1080/08870446.2015.1026904

Обратите внимание, представленные выше научные тексты размещены для ознакомления и получены посредством распознавания оригинальных текстов диссертаций (OCR). В связи с чем, в них могут содержаться ошибки, связанные с несовершенством алгоритмов распознавания. В PDF файлах диссертаций и авторефератов, которые мы доставляем, подобных ошибок нет.